In 2009-2010, I completed my sabbatical at the University of
California in Davis. In reality, however, much of my time was spent on my
family’s vineyard in Napa, California, where I lived for that year. (The vineyard and winery are owned by my uncle, George, and the vineyard manager is
my brother, Mike.) Before arriving in California, I had pledged to myself that
I would not start any new research projects but rather finish up some old ones
and start a book on Eco-Evolutionary Dynamics. Here is the story of how
serendipity – and perhaps a lot of fine wine – led me to break my vow.
Hendry Vineyard in winter (M. Hendry photo). |
Nearly every day, my kids (Aspen – 7 years old – and Cedar –
4 years old) and I would go for a stroll around the vineyard, taking pictures,
reminiscing about old family stories back to 1939 when my grandfather bought
the property, or just looking at the many critters that lived on the vineyard.
A few weeks into our stay, we found ourselves walking along the creek that
flows through the property. In fact, the creek does not actually flow during
the summer because it dries up except for a few spring-fed pools. As we came to
one of these pools, the kids got all excited about the small fish they could
see rushing around in what little water remained. “Catch the fish daddy, catch
the fish.” Well, it is hard to resist the kids when they want to catch fish,
and so we got some small nets and set to it. To my complete surprise, it turned
out that the most numerous fish in these tiny pools were threespine stickleback.
Hendry Vineyard and Winery in autumn (M. Hendry photo). |
Now I was hard pressed to keep to my vow to not start a new
research project given that stickleback are one of my major research topics. So
I spent the next few days drinking wine and mulling over various projects that
I might do, such as a study of how periodic drying into isolated pools shapes
the “metapopulation” structure of a creek stickleback population. However, I
eventually convinced myself that this would be forcing a new project that
really wasn’t that closely related to my research. So I decided to set it aside
and return to writing.
A few weeks later, one of our walks took us past the two reservoirs
on the property and I happened to look in and notice some small fish swimming
around. I looked closer – stickleback again! Now fate just seemed too obvious
to ignore – we were literally living between a reservoir and a creek, and my
stickleback research focuses on lake and stream populations. Moreover, the two
reservoirs had been created in the early 1970s by pumping water from the creek
– and this would have been how the stickleback colonized the reservoirs. So not
only was it a lake-stream stickleback pair in our backyard but it was also a
potential “rapid” evolution scenario – one of my other major research interests.
How could I not study it? In particular, we decided to test if adaptive
divergence had occurred between reservoir and creek stickleback in the 35 or so
years since the reservoirs were created – and whether any such divergence was
parallel (in the same direction) as that between the long-established
lake-stream pairs I had studied in British Columbia (for example, stream
stickleback essentially always have deeper bodies and fewer gill rakers than do
lake stickleback). If so, we would have a particularly striking demonstration
of the power, speed, and predictability of natural selection.
The creek is shown in the white line and the reservoirs in the white circles. |
Aspen and Cedar set and retrieved the minnow traps, Cedar
“died” the stickleback, I photographed them, and Aspen labeled and preserved
them. The next year back home in Montreal, we continued the family project on
rainy days and in the dead of winter. Aspen set the morphometric landmarks on
the computer, Cedar took the fish out of the vials, I measured and dissected
the fish (thanks to my mom donating her dissecting microscope), and Aspen
recorded the data in the computer and returned the stickleback to the vials.
The next year it was back to the vineyard for a second round of sampling and
then came another winter of fish processing. Now, with two years of data in
hand, it was time for some stats and writing, a task I will admit did not play
to the kids’ expertise. Just a few weeks ago, the paper resulting from our
project was published in Evolutionary
Ecology Research, and so we can now spill the beans on our discoveries.
Cedar out to catch some stickleback 2009. |
Aspen checks the traps 2009. |
Our first major finding was no noteworthy divergence between
creek and reservoir stickleback. Although this was initially disappointing, it
eventually became more exciting – because it represented a dramatic exception
to many other lake-stream pairs and to the frequent evidence for rapid
evolution in stickleback. In short, it would be of limited value to just
confirm what we already know about stickleback. Instead, the story became about
solving the riddle of what makes the Hendry Vineyard stickleback situation
unique. One likely possibility for the lack of creek-reservoir divergence is gene
flow that prevents the populations from evolving separately in response to
different selection regimes. And gene flow indeed could be high given that
water continues to be pumped each year from the creek into the reservoirs (although
filters are present and the pumps are probably not healthy for stickleback, some
must make it through). To gain some initial insight into this possibility, Ella
Bowles and Sean Rogers at the University of Calgary screen some of our samples
for genetic variation at DNA microsatellites and found very low divergence –
indicating high gene flow. So the story turns from one of the power, speed, and
predictability of natural selection to a demonstration of constraints and
limits on divergence.
Our second major finding was that morphological variation in
Hendry Vineyard stickleback – in both reservoirs and in the creek – was
extremely high. This was true for overall body shape and even more dramatically
so for mouth shape – a key aspect of stickleback adaptation. In fact,
consultation with many stickleback biologists suggests that the variation at
these sites was higher than that in any other known stickleback population. So this
will be our next project – to understand the origin and fate of morphological
variation, which might yield insight into how new traits can arise and spread
within populations.
This week, we embarked on the first phase our new project during
our annual visit to the vineyard. In reality, much of our time was spent
building a new horse fence and exploring the Hendry wine library (thankfully a “living
library” from which one periodically tests how the wines are ageing). However, we
did have a full day to start the new project by collecting, photographing, and
preserving 50 stickleback from one of the reservoirs. Over the next long, cold
Montreal winter, we will use these fish to see if morphological variation
(particularly the extremes of body and head shape) is associated with diet
variation, which could be one possible reason for why such extreme
morphological traits can arise and spread: that is, they are well suited for
particular food types. Of course, many other questions remain, including
whether these morphologies are genetically based or environmentally induced, a
question we will address on my next sabbatical by setting up a stickleback
breeding laboratory in a barn on the vineyard. Perhaps a stickleback library is
not as good as a wine library but who says one can’t have both.
__________________________
The paper:
Hendry, A.P., A.S. Hendry, and C.A. Hendry. 2013. Hendry vineyard stickleback:
testing for contemporary lake-stream divergence. Evolutionary Ecology Research 15:343-359. PDF
An earlier post about the Hendry Vineyard: The Kumbaya Model and the Jake Reset Effect
Have you checked if your kids are actually the youngest published scientific authors?
ReplyDeleteGuinness Book of World Records website state:
"At age 11, Emily Rosa, of Loveland, Colorado, USA, became the youngest person to have research published in a scientific or medical journal when an article she co-authored appeared in the Journal of the American Medical Association on 1 April 1998."
Then, of course, more recently there was the Blackawton Bee study by a class of 8-year olds published in Biology Letters, but your children seem to be even younger.
By the way, interesting story. It is nice to get to know the background to a paper. Makes you remember the research and the results.
It also made me think about some projects I could do. We have estuarine sticklebacks here in Gothenburg, Sweden. Some students measured spine length on two populations and saw that fish from a man-made brackish-water pond had longer spines than stream-captured fish (populations are only isolated by distance). I think the predation pressure, both from fish and birds is much greater in the pond (it's quite large). Haven't got around to test it properly, as I'm a PhD student and this is not really part of my project. However, our sticklebacks are also highly diverse in morphology - so perhaps I should into these populations as a side-project.
Thanks for the info. I was aware of the Bee study but I hadn't considered whether or not there was a record for such things. Aspen and Cedar were both under 8 when the work started but it wasn't published until just this year. Aspen is now 10 and Cedar 7 - so Cedar at least would still beat those Blackawton pretenders.
DeleteContemporary (rapid) evolution studies are fund regardless of their outcome - so make sure to sample those Swedish pond and stream stickleback!